5 Study “Hacks” That Actually Work (Backed by Science)
In the world of study advice, opinions are abundant but evidence is often scarce. Between social media "studytubers" showcasing aesthetic notes and well-meaning friends suggesting cramming sessions, it's hard to separate legitimate strategies from those that merely sound good.
This article cuts through the noise to deliver five study techniques with substantial scientific research backing their effectiveness. We're not just telling you what works—we're showing you why it works, based on peer-reviewed research and cognitive science principles.
1. Spaced Repetition: The Forgetting Curve Hack
The Science: In 1885, German psychologist Hermann Ebbinghaus pioneered research on memory and forgetting, discovering what we now call the "forgetting curve." His research demonstrated that without reinforcement, we forget approximately 70% of new information within 24 hours and nearly 90% within a week.
Multiple studies since then, including a comprehensive meta-analysis published in Psychological Science by Cepeda et al. (2008), have confirmed that spacing out study sessions significantly improves long-term retention compared to massed practice (cramming). The analysis of 254 studies found spacing effects improved learning outcomes by 15% to 63% depending on the material.
How It Works: Spaced repetition leverages the psychological spacing effect—the brain strengthens neural pathways each time you recall information, especially when that recall requires effort. The optimal approach involves reviewing material just as you're about to forget it, with gradually increasing intervals between review sessions.
Practical Application:
After learning new material, review it within 24 hours
Review again after 3 days
Review again after 1 week
Review again after 2 weeks
Review again after 1 month
Dr. Robert Bjork, Distinguished Research Professor at UCLA and memory expert, explains: "Spacing study sessions so that some forgetting is allowed to occur between them creates just enough 'desirable difficulty' to enhance learning."
Research-Based Evidence: A 2014 study published in Educational Psychology Review found medical students using spaced repetition retained approximately 35% more information over a six-month period compared to students using traditional study methods.
2. Retrieval Practice: The Testing Effect
The Science: One of the most robust findings in cognitive psychology is what researchers call the "testing effect"—the discovery that actively retrieving information from memory creates stronger learning than passive review. A landmark 2008 study in Science by Karpicke and Roediger demonstrated that students who practiced retrieving information remembered approximately 50% more material a week later than those who simply reviewed the material multiple times.
How It Works: According to cognitive psychologist Dr. Pooja Agarwal, "Retrieval practice strengthens the neural pathways associated with a memory, making it easier to access that information in the future." The effort required to recall information without looking at notes or textbooks creates stronger memory traces than re-reading or highlighting.
Practical Application:
Close your book and write down everything you remember about a topic
Use flashcards, but focus on active recall (not recognition)
Explain concepts out loud without referring to notes
Take practice tests under exam-like conditions
Use the Cornell Note-Taking System, which incorporates a retrieval column
A 2021 study in the Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition found that even when retrieval attempts are unsuccessful, the subsequent learning is enhanced—meaning that struggling to remember something (even if you get it wrong) improves your ability to learn it correctly afterward.
Research-Based Evidence: A meta-analysis of 118 studies published in Psychological Bulletin confirmed that retrieval practice produces better long-term retention than all other studied learning techniques, with an average 42% improvement across studies.
3. Interleaved Practice: The Brain's Contextual Challenge
The Science: Traditional blocked practice (studying one topic or problem type exhaustively before moving to the next) feels productive but produces weaker learning outcomes than interleaved practice (mixing different topics or problem types). Research published in Current Directions in Psychological Science by Rohrer and Pashler demonstrated that interleaving improved test scores by an average of 43% for mathematical problem-solving and 76% for artistic style identification.
How It Works: Interleaving forces the brain to continuously retrieve different strategies and solutions, strengthening discrimination between problem types and developing better selection of appropriate techniques. The cognitive challenge of switching between topics creates what neuroscientists call "contextual interference," which enhances memory formation and transfer of knowledge.
Practical Application:
Alternate between different types of math problems rather than completing all of one type
Rotate between different foreign language grammar topics within one study session
Switch between related but different subjects (e.g., 30 minutes of organic chemistry, then 30 minutes of biochemistry)
Create study sessions where you practice identifying which formulas or principles apply to different problems
Dr. Elizabeth Bjork of UCLA explains: "Interleaving creates desirable difficulties that lead to stronger learning. When practice is interleaved, you're continuously having to reload the appropriate strategy for each type of problem."
Research-Based Evidence: A 2019 study in the Journal of Educational Psychology demonstrated that students using interleaved practice in mathematics performed 25% better on a test one day later and an impressive 76% better when tested one month later compared to students using blocked practice.
4. Dual Coding: The Visual-Verbal Connection
The Science: Dual coding theory, first proposed by Allan Paivio in 1971 and supported by decades of subsequent research, demonstrates that memory significantly improves when information is processed both verbally and visually. A 2015 meta-analysis published in Educational Research Review found that combining verbal explanations with visual representations improved learning outcomes by an average of 34% across multiple disciplines.
How It Works: The brain processes and stores information in multiple interconnected systems. When you encode information both visually and verbally, you create multiple neural pathways to the same information. According to cognitive neuroscience, this redundancy significantly increases the likelihood of successful retrieval.
Practical Application:
Transform text notes into diagrams, flowcharts, or mind maps
Use visual metaphors to represent abstract concepts
Annotate images with verbal explanations
Create comparison charts for related concepts
Draw sketches to represent relationships between ideas (even simple ones)
Dr. Richard Mayer, Distinguished Professor of Psychology at the University of California, notes: "People learn better from words and pictures than from words alone. The combination allows the brain to build stronger mental models."
Research-Based Evidence: Research published in Learning and Instruction found that students who used both verbal and visual notes recalled 40% more information three weeks later than students who used only text-based notes.
5. The Feynman Technique: Teaching to Learn
The Science: Named after Nobel Prize-winning physicist Richard Feynman, this technique embodies what cognitive scientists call "elaborative interrogation" and "self-explanation"—processes proven to enhance learning significantly. A comprehensive meta-analysis published in Educational Psychology Review by Dunlosky et al. (2013) rated self-explanation as having "high utility" for learning across contexts and subject matters.
How It Works: The technique leverages the cognitive principle that explaining concepts forces deeper processing than passive review. When you attempt to teach something, you must restructure knowledge, identify gaps in understanding, and translate complex ideas into simpler terms—all processes that dramatically strengthen neural connections.
Practical Application:
Choose a concept and write an explanation as if teaching a 12-year-old
Identify gaps in your explanation (areas where you struggled or had to look up information)
Return to your source material to address those gaps
Simplify further and use analogies to clarify difficult points
Cognitive scientist Dr. Daniel Willingham explains: "The process of translating abstract concepts into concrete examples reveals whether you truly understand something or are simply parroting information."
Research-Based Evidence: A 2018 study in Applied Cognitive Psychology found that students who used the explanation technique scored approximately 24% higher on conceptual understanding tests than students who repeatedly studied the material using their preferred methods.
The Implementation Gap: Knowledge vs. Practice
Perhaps the most interesting scientific finding about these techniques is what researchers call the "utilization problem." Despite overwhelming evidence supporting their effectiveness, these methods are significantly underutilized by students.
A 2009 survey published in Contemporary Educational Psychology found that 84% of students reported using rereading as their primary study strategy, despite its relatively limited effectiveness compared to the techniques outlined above.
Dr. Stephen Chew, cognitive psychologist and expert on teaching and learning, points out: "Students often confuse familiarity with understanding. Rereading creates a sense of fluency that feels like learning but doesn't create durable knowledge."
The science is clear: implementing these evidence-based techniques requires more effort initially but dramatically improves long-term retention and understanding—exactly what's needed for cumulative learning disciplines and career success.
According to Dr. Henry Roediger, memory researcher and co-author of "Make It Stick: The Science of Successful Learning": "Learning is deeper and more durable when it's effortful. Learning that's easy is like writing in sand, here today and gone tomorrow."
These five techniques aren't just study hacks—they're applications of fundamental principles of cognitive science that transform how the brain encodes, stores, and retrieves information. The research doesn't just suggest they work; it demonstrates why and how dramatically they outperform conventional study methods.